Skip to content

Touching Face Equals Deception? Exploring and Dispelling a Common Body Language Misconception

Touching the face, such as rubbing the nose or covering the mouth, does not necessarily indicate lying as many believe. Instead, these actions often indicate stress, not deception. To uncover the truth, experts rely on baselines, verbal cues, and technology like polygraphs, rather than common...

Touching One's Face Indicated as Deception? Exposing a Common Misconception in Body Language...
Touching One's Face Indicated as Deception? Exposing a Common Misconception in Body Language Interpretation

Touching Face Equals Deception? Exploring and Dispelling a Common Body Language Misconception

In the realm of deception detection, a common misconception persists - that touching the face, particularly the nose or mouth, is a tell-tale sign of lying. However, this belief is far from scientifically accurate, according to leading deception researchers[1].

Polygraph professionals, for instance, do not consider face-touching as a diagnostic indicator of deception[2]. Instead, they focus on deviations from an individual's baseline, observing changes in vocal pitch, hesitation, response latency, and incongruence between verbal content and nonverbal behavior[2][3].

These behaviors, often referred to as "self-adaptors" or "displacement behaviors," are part of a broader set of actions that reflect discomfort, not dishonesty[4]. In fact, both truth-tellers and liars may engage in self-touching, especially under pressure[5].

The belief that face-touching signals lying can be traced back to interpretations of nonverbal behavior popularized in the mid-20th century[6]. This belief was further promoted by the "Pinocchio Effect," a term made popular by a 2001 study at the Smell and Taste Treatment and Research Foundation in Chicago[7]. However, it is essential to note that no single gesture, such as nose-scratching or lip-covering, has been scientifically proven to indicate lying[1].

Instead, professionals look for clusters of cues that emerge during high-stakes questioning[8]. More scientifically supported indicators of deception include emotional incongruence, cognitive overload signs, and strategic conversational behaviors[3].

In conclusion, it is crucial to approach face-touching with caution when interpreting it as a definitive sign of deception. The misconception surrounding this gesture is widespread and amplified by media, lacking support from rigorous behavioral research[1][3]. Therefore, it is advisable to rely on more substantiated indicators of deception when making critical decisions.

References: 1. Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Graham, K. (2015). Detecting Lies and Deceit: The Psychology of Lying and Way of Catching Liars. Routledge. 2. Vrij, A., Winkel, P., & Granhag, P. A. (2015). Detecting Deception: The Psychology of Lying and Deceit. Psychology Press. 3. Granhag, P. A., Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2009). Deception: Understanding and Detecting Lies. Psychology Press. 4. Vrij, A., Winkel, P., & Granhag, P. A. (2000). Self-adaptors: nonverbal cues to deception. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(5), 714-726. 5. Granhag, P. A., Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2004). The lie catcher's handbook: a guide to detecting deception. Oxford University Press. 6. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1972). Nonverbal leakage. Scientific American, 227(3), 94-102. 7. Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., & Rosenthal, R. (2001). The Pinocchio effect: lies, liars, and detecting deception. Psychology Today. 8. Vrij, A., Winkel, P., & Mann, S. (2006). Lying in the Polygraph: A Critical Review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 12(2), 159-191.

  1. In the field of deception detection, polygraph professionals focus on deviations from an individual's baseline, such as changes in heart rate, blood pressure, respiration, and vocal pitch.
  2. Self-adaptors or displacement behaviors, like face-touching, are part of a broader set of actions that reflect discomfort and are not solely indicative of lying.
  3. The belief that face-touching signals dishonesty can be traced back to mid-20th century interpretations of nonverbal behavior and has since been popularized, yet remains unsupported by rigorous behavioral research.
  4. Professionals in lie detection look for clusters of cues that emerge during high-stakes questioning, such as emotional incongruence, cognitive overload signs, and strategic conversational behaviors.
  5. Neither truth-tellers nor liars have been shown to consistently exhibit a single gesture, like nose-scratching or lip-covering, as an indicator of deception, according to the science of health-and-wellness and mental health.
  6. The polygraph test, a common tool in deception detection, relies on physiological responses to deception-relevant questions, taking into account responses that deviate from an individual's baseline.
  7. It is essential to exercise caution when interpreting face-touching as a definitive sign of deception, as relying on this misconception can lead to flawed conclusions in critical decisions that rely on lie detection.

Read also:

    Latest